Kullanım Kılavuzu
Neden sadece 3 sonuç görüntüleyebiliyorum?
Sadece üye olan kurumların ağından bağlandığınız da tüm sonuçları görüntüleyebilirsiniz. Üye olmayan kurumlar için kurum yetkililerinin başvurması durumunda 1 aylık ücretsiz deneme sürümü açmaktayız.
Benim olmayan çok sonuç geliyor?
Birçok kaynakça da atıflar "Soyad, İ" olarak gösterildiği için özellikle Soyad ve isminin baş harfi aynı olan akademisyenlerin atıfları zaman zaman karışabilmektedir. Bu sorun tüm dünyadaki atıf dizinlerinin sıkça karşılaştığı bir sorundur.
Sadece ilgili makaleme yapılan atıfları nasıl görebilirim?
Makalenizin ismini arattıktan sonra detaylar kısmına bastığınız anda seçtiğiniz makaleye yapılan atıfları görebilirsiniz.
 ASOS INDEKS
 Görüntüleme 23
An Investigation of Executive Function in Children with Dyslexia
2019
Dergi:  
Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences
Yazar:  
Özet:

INTRODUCTION Although specific learning disabilities have intact senses, a true motivation, average intelligence, and adequate socio-cultural level, it is a general term with significant, unexpected, specific and persistent neurobehavioral difficulties in learning which defines a heterogeneous group who have difficulty in active reading, writing, and calculating (1). Dyslexia is the most common type of specific learning disabilities (1,4). Substantial evidence has established that the children with dyslexia have deficits in phonologic awareness, which consistently distinguish them from those who are not reading-impaired (5) and the primary problems of the children with dyslexia are, language-based, reading and writing. However, dyslexia consist of multiple neurocognitive deficits and not solely related to a phonological system dysfunction, and parents and professionals (6) often report other symptoms such as lack of concentration, disorganization, and forgetfulness. Moreover, there are functional anomalies in medial and left lateral frontal lobe reported in children with dyslexia (6–9). Thus, the executive functions have been explored as a contributing factor to dyslexia recently (1,10–14). Executive functions are high-level cognitive skills that control other functions and behaviors (13,15). These include skills such as planning, problem-solving, working memory, organization, cognitive flexibility and inhibition. Executive functions which play an essential role in the child's behavior, emotional regulation, and social interactions develop during childhood and adolescence (1,6). There are some assessment techniques such as record review, direct observation, interview, questionnaires, and rating scales that are structured and flexible as they pertain to EF. Context is critical when assessing for EF and EdF (6). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function or BRIEF is the test that aims to evaluate a child's executive functioning behaviors in a real-world setting (16). The BRIEF has provided an alternative to prevailing neuropsychological tests (17). The traditional neuropsychological tests are administered under controlled laboratory conditions while the BRIEF measures a child's executive functioning behaviors in everyday situations and settings and uniquely evaluate a set of metacognitive, behavioral, and emotional abilities that go beyond common psychopathology and behavioral disturbances measured (17). Vriezen and Pigott found that among children with traumatic brain injury (TBI), BRIEF may show the difficulties in everyday life than on any other performance-based neuropsychological tests that children can perform relatively well (18). The authors suggest that the BRIEF measures a unique aspect of executive functioning. Executive dysfunctions are described in a variety of developmental disorders such as ADHD, autism spectrum disorder (ASD), reading disabilities and TBI (19–23). Information processing, recalling the steps that will take place during a daily life mission are the problematic executive functions in dyslexia (10,24–26). As seen in the literature, although there are many studies about the problems of the executive functions of children with dyslexia, there is no consensus on the problems of daily life behavior caused by executive function problems in children with dyslexia. Therefore, in this present study we aimed to assess various aspects of executive functioning of children with dyslexia. In addition, we aimed to determine the differences between the dyslexic children and a control group in their executive functions. The light of this information will provide a more detailed understanding of the executive functioning of children with dyslexia as well as an increased chance of a targeted educational program. METHODS Study Design and Participants This study was a cross-sectional study which was carried out by using a questionnaire that was given to 158 parents of children with dyslexia (55 females and 103 males; mean age, 10.48±2.37 years; age range, 8-17 years) who applied to Hacettepe University, School of Health Sciences, Department of Occupational Therapy for their children. Children who were diagnosed with dyslexia according to the DSM-IV criteria among the children who presented to Hacettepe University Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinic between the years of 2015 and 2017 were selected by simple randomization method. At the beginning of the study, children and their parents were informed about the study and a child consent form and a parental consent form which were found to be convenient by Hacettepe University Ethics Committee were signed, and it was found appropriate from the viewpoint of medical ethics. Inclusion criteria; children who were diagnosed with dyslexia, were between the ages of 8-17, going to school in general education institutions (criteria for the dyslexia group), those continuing in the special education institution (criteria for the dyslexia group) and were dominant in the right hand. A score below 90 in the total intelligence score determined by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) test (the criterion for the children with dyslexia) and accompany of other physical, neurological and psychological problems were the exclusion criteria for children. For the children with dyslexia, there were 162 children between the ages of 8-17, attending the special education program and who have been shown not to have any physical, neurological and psychological problem and their families have been interviewed. Of the interviewed children, 160 have met the inclusion criteria. One hundred fifty-eight signed the consent forms by agreeing to participate in the study, and they were evaluated in the pediatric unit of Occupational Therapy Department of Hacettepe University. As the control group, 167 children with typical development were included with similar age and gender from the schools selected by the Directorate with the permission of the Ministry of National Education. “The Demographic Information form” and “Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) teacher and parents form” assess children's executive functions were used and was filled by children's teachers and parents (27,28). Instruments Demographic Information: A demographic information form prepared by the researchers by taking a comprehensive story from the children and the age, gender, grade of the child, the age of the mother and father, and the educational status of the mother and father were recorded. Evaluating Executive Functions: The BRIEF was used to evaluate executive functions. This scale is an inventory that measures the daily behaviors of executive functioning of the children and adolescents aged between 5 to 18 years with the teacher, parents, and self (12-18 years) evaluation form and measured across eight domains/subscales (Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Organization of Materials, and Monitor) (6,28). It had 86 items, and a three-point scale was used with indications of ‘Never,' ‘Sometimes,' ‘Often,' and respondents who at least were the fifth-grade education level found it easy to complete with the straight forward instructions. Approximately 10 to 15 minutes was enough to complete each questionnaire and a hidden carbon copy that simplifies later addition of item scores within each scale for computation of T scores was also included. The Turkish version of the test was made by Batan et al. (27). High internal consistency (alphas=0.80-0.98); test-retest reliability (r=0.82 for parents and 0,88 for teachers); and moderate correlations between teacher and parent ratings (r=0.32-0.34) were computed for original forms. Statistical Analysis IBM SPSS statistics for Windows 23.0 program (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 23.0. Armonk, New York, United States of America) was used for statistical analysis of the data. Power analysis was applied to determine the appropriate number of samples. The sample size of this study was calculated by taking the effect size of the study as 0.50, with 95% power and at least 150 children per group with 0.05 α error. Cronbach's alpha coefficients were computed to assess the reliability of the BRIEF parents and teacher form (29). Percentage values for qualitative variables were calculated (Table 1), and the variables determined by measurement were expressed as the mean standard deviation (X±SD) (Table 2) (30). The Shapiro Wilks test was used to determine whether the distribution of the numerical variables was normal. Since this test provided a normal distribution assumption, the independent-sample t-test was used to compare the mean of the groups (30). RESULTS Age, gender, parental education and work status of the parents of the children were compared, and all the characteristics of the groups were found to be similar except maternal education and maternal employment status (p>0.05) (Table 1). When we compared the groups in terms of the total and sub-scores of the BRIEF-teacher and parents form that we used for the evaluation of executive functions, the average of the executive functions of the children with dyslexia regarding inhibition, shift, emotional regulation, initiation, memory, plan, organization and monitor parameters were found very low and the difference between the groups was statistically significant (p=0.001). Moreover, emotional regulation (Table 2 and 3). Children with dyslexia were found to be negatively affected by these parameters compared to children with typical development. DISCUSSION In this study, we examined the executive functional problems of children with dyslexia, and it was found that children with dyslexia were behind the normal development group in terms of executive functions such as planning, organization, inhibition, monitor, initiation, working memory, and emotional regulation. As it is seen in many studies the most commonly studied parameter of executive functions in children with dyslexia is working memory (31–38). The general conclusions of these studies were that children with dyslexia had deterioration in complex visual-spatial (36) or auditory tasks (counting number) (37,38), which requires storage and processing skills. Also, these studies confirmed that children with dyslexia had more problems in the memory task than the controls (31). These findings suggested that children with dyslexia may have been able to keep visual and auditory information in their memory for a shorter period than children with typical development. According to these results, task memory and recall mechanism were found to be affected in children with dyslexia. This finding was also supported in the present study we performed. Children without dyslexia scored higher in the memory parameters than children with dyslexia. Notably, the questions "somethings are difficult to remember even for a few minutes, and they cannot talk on the same topic for a long time" were the questions that the group of children with dyslexia scored the lowest for the task memory parameter. There are inconsistent results in the literature related to the evaluation of impulse control ability (inhibition) (10,39,40). Tasks related to impulse control and flexibility can be straightforward for children with dyslexia, but there are still more studies pointing out the impulse control disorder in children with dyslexia compared to children without dyslexia, and this was also consistent with our results (39,40). In this study behaviors related to impulse control with a high incidence were "do not think about the results before doing something" and "talking in inappropriate times." Problem solving and planning and organizational skills are the main elements of executive functions. Although previous studies that question the integrity of these skills in children with dyslexia are inconsistent, most studies have reported significant differences in problem-solving between children with and without dyslexia (41,42). The literature, especially, reveals the significant differences between the duration of solving a problem and the number of problems; also, the study reveals that control group showed significantly better behavioral patterns related to problem-solving (10). In this study, typically developed children demonstrated significantly better behaviors of planning and organizing skills. Also, the most problematic planning and organizational behaviors on the scale were; "cannot be creative in organizing," "cannot properly adjust the time that is required to fulfill the tasks assigned to him” and “does not come prepared to class." A component of executive functions, attention shift, is a problematic area in children and adults with dyslexia and our result is consistent with these findings (43–45). It is reported in the literature that this problem arises from the source of auditory perception problem (46). However, it has been described many times that the main problem is visual with dyslexia (47–49). In this present study, the most problematic behavior related to attention shifting that is seen in children with dyslexia was “difficulty in performing an activity with multiple stages" The problem about attention shifting is visual or auditory, or whether the same problem causes both is a matter to investigate. Emotional regulation, a significant problem seen in children with dyslexia, is also a component of executive functions (50,51). However, in the current literature, researchers are predicting that problems in emotional regulation should not be seen as a reason for dyslexia, and not be kept ahead of the problematic cognitive processes. Besides, it is also thought that people with dyslexia should be under control during studies on the grounds of contradictory data on their emotional states (50). There is also a significant difference in emotional regulation among children with and without dyslexia in this study. At this precise point in this study, problematic behaviors related to emotional regulation in children with dyslexia were "be frustrated for a long time after experiencing a problem" and "very easily be upset." In this context, it was thought that emotional regulation is vital in the lives of these children and it should take place in therapies. Some studies examined the effect of self-monitoring over the reading performance on children with dyslexia, and in the literature, it has been reported that self-monitoring, decreases errors (52) and improves reading behavior and academic performance (53). Consistent with the literature, in this study, there was a significant difference in monitor behaviors of children with dyslexia compared to those who without dyslexia. Teachers of children with dyslexia have expressed this difficulty by indicating the behaviors such as "do not realize their behavior in a group" and parents expressed for this "do not realize that their behavior causes negative reactions." As a result, when comparing the parameters of executive functions between children with and without dyslexia, there was a meaningful difference against children with dyslexia regarding working memory, impulse control, shift, problem-solving, organization, emotional regulation, and monitoring. The present results are valuable because a control group monitors the groups with similar in age and sex. However, the Intelligence Quotient (IQ) level of the control group was not examined and can be seen as a limitation due to the fact that the groups were not homogenized according to the IQ score. Future studies should also examine the impact of IQ situations on executive functioning. Also examining executive functions with an inventory instead of psychometric tests may be a limitation in this study. For this reason, it is important that future studies should be conducted under more favorable conditions with psychometric tests. Nevertheless, the high number of participants and the fact that this is a randomized controlled study design, this study may help shed light on the everyday and academic problems of children with dyslexia. CONCLUSIONS In conclusion, executive functions may affect not only attention and memory in children with dyslexia but also behavior and interactions of the child negatively. Hence children with dyslexia may have great difficulty in developing these skills spontaneously. Executive functions have an essential contribution to both daily and school life, and it is essential to evaluate executive functions for the development of therapy programs for children with dyslexia and as well as implementing effective targeted treatment strategies. Ethics Committee Approval: This study was evaluated by the Ethics Committee of Hacettepe University at 16-375 application number at the meeting held on 31.05.2016 and it was found appropriate from the viewpoint of medical ethics. Conflict of Interest: There is no conflict of interest with any financial organization regarding the materials discussed in the manuscript. Financial Support: The authors declared that this study has received no financial support. REFERENCES 1.Taur S, Karande S, Saxena AA, Gogtay NJ, Thatte UM. Use of computerized tests to evaluate psychomotor performance in children with specific learning disabilities in comparison to normal children. Indian J Med Res 2014;140(5):644-48. 2.Korkmazlar U. Özel öğrenme bozukluğu (6-11 yaş ilkokul çocuklarında özel öğrenme bozukluğu ve tanı yöntemleri). İstanbul Taçe Ofset; 1993. [Turkish] 3.Mammarella IC, Cornoldi C. An analysis of the criteria used to diagnose children with Nonverbal Learning Disability (NLD). Child Neuropsychol 2014;20(3):255-80. 4.Gooch D, Snowling M, Hulme C. Time perception, phonological skills and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011;52(2):195-203. 5.Demonet JF, Taylor MJ, Chaix Y. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 2004;363:1451-60. 6.Akyurek G. Executive Functions and Neurology in Children and Adolescents. In: M Huri, eds. Occupational Therapy-Therapeutic and Creative Use of Activity. IntechOpen; 2018. p. 31-49. 7.Zadina JN, Corey DM, Casbergue RM, Lemen LC, Rouse JC, Knaus TA, et al. Lobar asymmetries in subtypes of dyslexic and control subjects. J Child Neurol 2006;21(11): 922-31. 8.Menghini D, Finzi A, Benassi M, Bolzani R, Facoetti A, Giovagnoli S, et al. Different underlying neurocognitive deficits in developmental dyslexia: a comparative study. Neuropsychologia 2010;48(4):863-72. 9.Varvara P, Varuzza C, Padovano Sorrentino AC, Vicari S, Menghini D. Executive functions in developmental dyslexia. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8:120. 10.Reiter A, Tucha O, Lange KW. Executive functions in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia 2005;11(2):116-31. 11.Altemeier L, Jones J, Abbott RD, Berninger VW. Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: Note taking and report writing in third and fifth graders. Dev Neuropsychol 2006;29(1):161-73. 12.Cutting LE, Materek A, Cole CA, Levine TM, Mahone EM. Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performance. Ann Dyslexia 2009;59(1):34-54. 13.Akyurek G, Efe A, Kilic BG, Bumin G. The Effect of Cognitive Therapy on Executive Functions and Occupational Routines in Children with Dyslexia. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2018;99(10):e19. 14.Akyurek G, Bumin G. Home literacy environment and occupational routines in children with dyslexia. Bozok Medical Journal 2018;8(3):16-23. [Turkish] 15.Welsh MC, Pennington BF, Groisser DB. A normative‐developmental study of executive function: a window on prefrontal function in children. Dev Neuropsychol 1991;7(2):131-49. 16.Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Test review Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol 2000;6(3):235-8. 17.Donders J. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function: Introduction. Child Neuropsychol 2002;8:229-30. 18.Vriezen ER, Pigott SE. The relationship between parental report on the BRIEF and performance-based measures of executive function in children with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Child Neuropsychol 2002;8:296-303. 19.Kenworthy LE, Black DO, Wallace GL, Ahluvalia T, Wagner AE, Sirian LM. Disorganization: the forgotten executive dysfunction in high-functioning autism (HFA) spectrum disorders. Dev Neuropsychol 2005;28(3):809-27. 20.Robinson S, Goddard L, Dritschel B, Wisley M, Howlin P. Executive functions in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Cogn 2009;71(3):362-8. 21.Corbett BA, Constantine LJ, Hendren R, Rocke D, Ozonoff S. Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Res 2009;166(2-3):210-22. 22.Abad-Mas L, Ruiz-Andres R, Moreno-Madrid F, Sirera-Conca MA., Cornesse M, Delgado-Mejia ID, et al. Executive function training in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Rev Neurologia 2011;52:S77-83. 23.Pani P, Menghini D, Napolitano C, Calcagni M, Armando M, Sergeant JA, et al. Proactive and reactive control of movement are differently affected in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder children. Int Res Dev Disabil 2013;34(10):3104-11. 24.Vakil E, Lowe M, Goldfus C. Performance of Children With Developmental Dyslexia on Two Skill Learning Tasks—Serial Reaction Time and Tower of Hanoi Puzzle: A Test of the Specific Procedural Learning Difficulties Theory. J Learn Disabil 2015; 48(5):471-81. 25.Horowitz‐Kraus T, Vannest JJ, Kadis D, Cicchino N, Wang YY, Holland SK. Reading acceleration training changes brain circuitry in children with reading difficulties. Brain Behav 2014;4(6):886-902. 26.Moura O, Simões MR, Pereira M. Working memory in Portuguese children with developmental dyslexia. Appl Neuropsychol Child 2015;4(4):237-48. 27.Batan SN, Oktem-Tanor O, Kalem E. Reliability and validity studies of Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a Turkish normative sample. Elementary Education Online 2011;10(3):894-904. 28.Akyurek G, Bumin G, Karabulut E. Reliability and validity of the behavior rating inventory of executive functions questionnaire in dyslexia. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2018;99(10):e20. 29.Kilic S. Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient. Journal of Mood Disorders 2016;6(1):47-8. [Turkish] 30.Altman DG, Gore SM, Gardner MJ, Pocock SJ. Statistical guidelines for contributors to medical journals. Br Med J 1983:7;1489-93. 31.Smith-Spark JH, Fisk JE. Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory 2007;15(1):34-56. 32.Berninger VW, Raskind W, Richards T, Abbott R, Stock P. A multidisciplinary approach to understanding developmental dyslexia within working-memory architecture: Genotypes, phenotypes, brain, and instruction. Dev Neuropsychol 2008;33(6):707-44. 33.Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev Psychol 2013;49(2):270-91. 34.Gathercole SE, Alloway TP, Willis C, Adams AM. Working memory in children with reading disabilities. J Exp Child Psychol 2006;93(3):265-81. 35.Alloway TP. Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children with learning difficulties. Eur J Psychol Assess 2009;25(2):92-8. 36.Weber RC, Riccio CA, Cohen MJ. Does Rey Complex Figure copy performance measure executive function in children? Appl Neuropsychol Child 2013;2(1):6-12. 37.Ackerman PT, Dykman RA, Gardner MY. Counting rate, naming rate, phonological sensitivity, and memory span: major factors in dyslexia. J Learn Disabil 1990;23(5): 325-7. 38.Helland T, Asbjornsen A. Digit span in dyslexia: variations according to language comprehension and mathematics skills. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004;26(1):31-42. 39.Borella E, Ghisletta P, De Ribaupierre A. Age differences in text processing: the role of working memory, inhibition, and processing speed. J Gerontol B: Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2011;66(3):311-20. 40.Wang LC, Tasi HJ, Yang HM. Cognitive inhibition in students with and without dyslexia and dyscalculia. Res Dev Disabil 2012;33(5):1453-61. 41.Newman SD, Carpenter PA, Varma S, Just MA. Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia 2003;41(12):1668-82. 42.Sullivan JR, Riccio CA, Castillo CL. Concurrent validity of the tower tasks as measures of executive function in adults: a meta-analysis. Appl Neuropsychol 2009;16(1):62-75. 43.Harrar V, Tammam J, Pérez-Bellido A, Pitt A, Stein J, Spence C. Multisensory integration and attention in developmental dyslexia. Curr Biol 2014;24(5):531-5. 44.Buchholz J, Davies AA. Adults with dyslexia demonstrate space-based and object-based covert attention deficits: shifting attention to the periphery and shifting attention between objects in the left visual field. Brain Cogn 2005;57(1):30-4. 45.Facoetti A, Lorusso ML, Paganoni P, Cattaneo C, Galli R, Umilta C, et al. Auditory and visual automatic attention deficits in developmental dyslexia. Cogn Brain Res 2003;16(2):185-91. 46.Hari R, Valta M, Uutela K. Prolonged attentional dwell time in dyslexic adults. Neurosci Lett 1999; 271(3): 202-204. 47.Giovagnoli G, Vicari S, Tomassetti S, Menghini D. The role of visual-spatial abilities in dyslexia: age differences in children’s reading? Front Psychol 2016;7:1-9. 48.Gabrieli JD, Norton ES. Reading abilities: Importance of visual-spatial attention. Curr Biol 2012;22(9):R298-R9. 49.Facoetti A, Corradi N, Ruffino M, Gori S, Zorzi M. Visual spatial attention and speech segmentation are both impaired in preschoolers at familial risk for developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 2010;16(3):226-39. 50.Riddick B. Living with dyslexia: the social and emotional consequences of specific learning difficulties/disabilities. London and New York: Routledge/Falmer, 2012. 51.Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ, Dean P. Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci 2001;24(9):508-11. 52.Cole CL, Bambara LM. Issues surrounding the use of self-management interventions in the schools. School Psychol Review 1992;21:193-201. 53.DiGangi SA, Maag JW, Rutherford Jr RB. Self-graphing of on-task behavior: enhancing the reactive effects of self-monitoring on on-task behavior and academic performance. Learn Disabil Q 1991;14(3):221-30.

Anahtar Kelimeler:

Atıf Yapanlar
Bilgi: Bu yayına herhangi bir atıf yapılmamıştır.
Benzer Makaleler








Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences

Alan :   Sağlık Bilimleri

Dergi Türü :   Uluslararası

Metrikler
Makale : 92
Atıf : 10
2023 Impact/Etki : 0.029
Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences